Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorKominsky, JF
dc.contributor.authorGerstenberg, T
dc.contributor.authorPelz, M
dc.contributor.authorSheskin, M
dc.contributor.authorSingmann, H
dc.contributor.authorSchulz, L
dc.contributor.authorKeil, FC
dc.date.accessioned2022-02-01T15:23:05Z
dc.date.available2022-02-01T15:23:05Z
dc.date.issued2021-02-01
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/139823
dc.description.abstract© 2021 American Psychological Association Young children often struggle to answer the question “what would have happened?” particularly in cases where the adult-like “correct” answer has the same outcome as the event that actually occurred. Previous work has assumed that children fail because they cannot engage in accurate counterfactual simulations. Children have trouble considering what to change and what to keep fixed when comparing counterfactual alternatives to reality. However, most developmental studies on counterfactual reasoning have relied on binary yes/no responses to counterfactual questions about complex narratives and so have only been able to document when these failures occur but not why and how. Here, we investigate counterfactual reasoning in a domain in which specific counterfactual possibilities are very concrete: simple collision interactions. In Experiment 1, we show that 5- to 10-year-old children (recruited from schools and museums in Connecticut) succeed in making predictions but struggle to answer binary counterfactual questions. In Experiment 2, we use a multiple-choice method to allow children to select a specific counterfactual possibility. We find evidence that 4- to 6-year-old children (recruited online from across the United States) do conduct counterfactual simulations, but the counterfactual possibilities younger children consider differ from adult-like reasoning in systematic ways. Experiment 3 provides further evidence that young children engage in simulation rather than using a simpler visual matching strategy. Together, these experiments show that the developmental changes in counterfactual reasoning are not simply a matter of whether children engage in counterfactual simulation but also how they do so. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved)en_US
dc.language.isoen
dc.publisherAmerican Psychological Association (APA)en_US
dc.relation.isversionof10.1037/dev0001140en_US
dc.rightsCreative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-Share Alikeen_US
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/en_US
dc.sourcePMCen_US
dc.titleThe trajectory of counterfactual simulation in development.en_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.identifier.citationKominsky, JF, Gerstenberg, T, Pelz, M, Sheskin, M, Singmann, H et al. 2021. "The trajectory of counterfactual simulation in development.." Developmental Psychology, 57 (2).
dc.contributor.departmentMassachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences
dc.contributor.departmentCenter for Brains, Minds, and Machines
dc.relation.journalDevelopmental Psychologyen_US
dc.eprint.versionAuthor's final manuscripten_US
dc.type.urihttp://purl.org/eprint/type/JournalArticleen_US
eprint.statushttp://purl.org/eprint/status/PeerRevieweden_US
dc.date.updated2022-02-01T15:18:42Z
dspace.orderedauthorsKominsky, JF; Gerstenberg, T; Pelz, M; Sheskin, M; Singmann, H; Schulz, L; Keil, FCen_US
dspace.date.submission2022-02-01T15:18:44Z
mit.journal.volume57en_US
mit.journal.issue2en_US
mit.licenseOPEN_ACCESS_POLICY
mit.metadata.statusAuthority Work and Publication Information Neededen_US


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record